Tuesday, January 18, 2011

Do Men and Women Read Emotions Differently?



Appropriate social and emotional responses are often culturally driven, and produced to various degrees dependent on personality. Regardless of cultural association, some people, both male and female, seem more emotionally expressive and perceptive when compared to others—is there a biological explanation for this?

Possibly. But not in the way you would think.
ResearchBlogging.org

Emotions are a conscious experience. They're physical and we know they're happening. Even when we incorrectly identify them (e.g., saying "I'm not angry!" when in fact you're furious), we still experience them. But researchers Winkielman and Berridge (2004) have suggested that in some cases emotional processes may be unconscious, or implicit—that is we may feel something without being aware that we're having the feeling or behaving in a certain way. We may be influenced by subliminal stimuli. For example, research participants were exposed to several expressive faces and asked to then either pour themselves a drink or rate the drink. After viewing happy expressions, participants were more likely to drink more (they poured themselves larger drinks) and pay more for their drink (they rated the drink higher), especially if they were in fact thirsty (122).  Participants were asked to rate their own mood and reported no changes, which suggests no awareness about changes in mood. This consequently implies that they were swayed by subliminal messaging.

Winkielman and Berridge argue that the evolutionary purpose of emotions is to regulate appropriate responses—it helps us negotiate our networks:
Basic affective reactions are widely shared by animals, including reptiles and fish, and at least in some species may not involve conscious awareness comparable to that in humans. The original function of emotion was to allow the organism to react appropriately to positive or negative events, and conscious feelings might not always have been required (2004: 122).
Basic affective responses, such as liking pleasant experiences or feeling fear in threatening situations, may be hardwired into our social circuitry. These responses are controlled by subcortical structures in the brain—such as the amygdala—which carry out preconscious operations. Anencephalic infants, for example, who possess only a brainstem still demonstrate positive reactions to agreeable experiences like tasting sugar, and negative reactions to tasting bitter items.

The role the amygdala plays in emotional response is not fully understood. As discussed above, there may be some connection between the amygdala and basic affective responses. Anderson and Phelps (2000) presented a case study of a patient known as SP who suffered from lesions in the region of the amydala. In tests that asked her to identify the emotions of others, she demonstrated a diminished sensitivity to interpreting disgust and happiness:
Across patients, however, damage to the amygdala is most associated with impairments in the recognition of fear. Further, SP exhibited a pattern of impairment in the evaluation of expressions other than fear that is largely consistent with her extra-amygdalar damage in the right anteromedial temporal lobe. Thus, we conservatively assert that SP's deficits in recognizing expressions of fear are associated with lesions of her amygdala (Anderson and Phelps 2000: 108).
There is some belief that the amygdala in particular may play a role in our ability to learn and interpret nonverbal social communication. Both Winkielman and Berridge (2004) and Anderson and Phelps (2000) make reference to the importance for nonhuman primates to recognize social displays of fear and suggest that these responses may be based in this structure: "fearful facial expressions may be important for learning to fear previously neutral environmental stimuli" (Anderson and Phelps 2000: 111).

However, I'm going to work with the assumption that both the subway man and my clueless friend have fully functional amydaloid regions. It's possible that there is a gender difference when it comes to emotional understanding and response. A recent article from Hoffman and colleagues (2010) reports that women are better at interpreting subtle emotional clues. When asked to appropriately label facially expressed emotions, women were significantly better at picking up on low intensity emotional cues, so a glance or a movement may be significant to woman in a way that's very different to a man. In light of the discussion by Winkielman and Berridge, I'll venture to say that women may be more likely to respond to subliminal stimuli as well. Men appear to have trouble distinguishing anger and sadness from each other at low intensity expressions—so men aren't likely to pick up that you're upset if you're silently fuming, ladies.

The reasons for this difference isn't clear. There isn't a clear biological divide as to why women may be more perceptive than men when it comes to emotions. Certainly socialization may play a role in emotional sensitivity. But is that all there is to it? Perhaps emotional sensitivity plays a role in our evolutionary history—in hierarchical groups, it would have been beneficial to be able to read the social cues of others, particularly when they ranked higher within the group. It could have potentially saved the lives of offspring, and assured continued group membership and protection. For example, knowing when to avoid the group's matriarch or the Alpha male, could have been useful knowledge to have.

In this case, and in my own experience, these studies highlight that that there is in fact the potential for a biological and social divide in communication between the sexes that goes beyond simple socialization. Again, this is not to suggest that all men are emotional turnips or that women are all hypersensitive and savvy to emotions, but it does advocate for a bit of patience on both of our parts—particularly in public confrontations.

--
Photo by Kat Jayne from Pexels

Referenced:
Anderson, A., & Phelps, E. (2000). Expression Without Recognition: Contributions of the Human Amygdala to Emotional Communication Psychological Science, 11 (2), 106-111 DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00224

Hoffmann, H., Kessler, H., Eppel, T., Rukavina, S., & Traue, H. (2010). Expression intensity, gender and facial emotion recognition: Women recognize only subtle facial emotions better than men☆ Acta Psychologica, 135 (3), 278-283 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.012

Winkielman, P., & C. Berridge, K. (2004). Unconscious Emotion Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13 (3), 120-123 DOI: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00288.x

6 comments:

  1. I'm not sure if this is relevant, but I have a friend who is straight but is always assumed to be gay. We discussed this once and he said that he thought it was because he's an actor, and as an actor he deals in emotions, and that is read as gay.

    Now there are many assumptions in there, including the one that gay men deal with emotions differently than straight men do. However, IF indeed gay men are more sensitive to emotions, it might fit in with the theory that less powerful people need to be expert at reading the emotions of more powerful people--for example, when dealing with homophobia.

    ReplyDelete
  2. Some children who are bullied are bullied precisely because they aren't able to "read" others. This discussion makes me think of theory of mind - our ability to judge the intentions and motivations of others. It is an essential social development. I wonder if emotions may be linked to theory of mind faculties.

    Readers, do you know of any literature that addresses this?

    ReplyDelete
  3. Sometimes I wonder if such study of emotions is biased toward expressive emotional reaction. A frequent "male" response to upsetting stimuli is to dissociate emotionally.

    I recall last year, watching the movie "Sita Sings the Blues", and noting that it managed to show the male characters all acting in ways that betrayed that they were profoundly upset - but most of the commentators, generally female, were completely oblivious to it.

    I think it should be remembered first, that "inexpressive" does not mean "unemotional" and second that not everyone will be emotionally affected by the same things.

    ReplyDelete
  4. Agreed: "I think it should be remembered first, that "inexpressive" does not mean "unemotional" and second that not everyone will be emotionally affected by the same things."

    In the instances discussed in my post, however, we were dealing with cases where one individual just seemed utterly baffled by the emotional response/actions of the other individual in the engagement. So it was more than just a social or cultural response (which is how I'm interpreting your report of male dissociation), but just an individual who seemed to not understand the emotional state of the other person present.

    I can't speak for the couple on the train (I don't even know if they were/are a couple), but when I levied my criticism at my friend, I was speaking from a sense of frustration that he didn't seem to understand that I was upset. I sensed that frustration in the subway encounter, which prompted this post. But your latter point that not everyone is emotionally affected in the same way is a good one to bear in mind here. We all have different triggers, which is what makes these studies so difficult to apply to specific example in my opinion.

    ReplyDelete
  5. Well, speaking as a male who is often considered "cold", I'm sometimes frustrated by my SO who, if she us presented with something upsetting, finds it necessary to spend a significant period of time "being upset" before she's willing to address the issue or do anything except the activity of being upset.

    To me that's at best unproductive, and usually counterproductive. I'm saying (usually not out loud) "Okay, you got a bill you don't have the money for, let's see about payment plans. Let's find a solution." and she's raging about the unfairness of it all. Which is to say that someone who isn't emotionally expressive, or doesn't get emotionally involved in events, is often able to think rationally and act in a productive manner.

    But then, if that's the end of it, why do we get upset at all? How is that a useful behavior? I'm reasonably sure it is, because otherwise we would learn to not do it. I think it is, consciously or not, intended as a display for others. It discourages others from providing more of whatever triggered the upset/anger response; and it might cause other people to devote their resources to mitigating or removing the response in the first place. Of course, this is only effective if other people know *why* the response came about.

    ReplyDelete
  6. Why DO we get upset all? Particularly over things we can't control? Anger must have some sort of regulatory purpose—I'm reminded of the Simpson's episode where Flanders finally erupts and speaks gibberish, and is told that this is the result of everything being O-diddily-kay all the time. I think there's something to be said for being able to rationally discuss things that make you upset--as I tried to point out with my Angry Birds post. Of course, this depends on the other person being willing to participate in the discussion in the first place. So my question is why are we taught shy away from anger instead of meeting it head on and dealing with it productively?

    ReplyDelete